Summary

WAIS

Raven’s Progressive Matrices

Brain size

Evolution of Sex Differences in Intelligence 

 

 

Summary

-IQ of an adult male 4 to 5 points higher
-Faster development for women, but peaks earlier (parallel to physical growth)
-Cerebral volume peaks at age 10.5 in females and 14.5 in males
-IQ of men becomes superior at about 16 years
-Adult men brain bigger by 110 to 150 cubic cm (δ = 0.78 SD)
-19.3 billion neurons in women, 22.8 billion neurons in men
-Highest cortical thickness in men (δ = 0.28SD)
-Men have quicker Reaction Time (Deary, 2006 Ritchie, 2018) a consequence of the positive correlation between IQ and reaction time; Men also have a faster nerve conduction velocity (De Bellis, Keshavan, Beers et al. (2001)), consequence of the positive correlation between IQ and NCV.

Fig. 1. Total sample male and female general intelligence g (HFA/S–L, see text) distributions and male–female ratio as a function of male g = .23 (SD = 1.03) and female g = .23 (SD = .93). Nmales = 90 (mean age 13.0, SD = 3.54) and Nfemales = 91 (mean age 12.8, SD = 3.6).

The brain size of adult men is about 110 cc (0.78 SD) larger. The IQ-brain size correlation is +0.35 so the theoretical male superiority in adulthood is (0.78×0.35) = 0.273SD, ie in IQ points: (0.273×15) = 4 IQ points, exactly what is found empirically in the tests.

Up to the age of 15 years, males and females have approximately the same intelligence, except for a small male advantage on the visualization abilities; however, from the age of 16 years, males begin to show greater intelligence, reaching an advantage of from three to five IQ points in adults. This has been further confirmed by Paul Irwing and myself (Irwing & Lynn, 2005; Irwing, 2012), by Victoria Bourne, Helen Fox, Ian Deary, and Lawrence Whalley (2007).

This advantage is fully explicable by the larger size of mens’ brain. Men and women experience the same environment and therefore environmental factors can not account for the difference between men and women. The evolutionary  explanation of the average slightly higher intelligence of men is that men compete with each other to obtain women and in the evolution of hominids, the intelligence played an important role. Women do not compete for men.

Summary on Sex Differences in Intelligence by Richard Lynn (2017).

 

WAIS (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale)

All 42 studies show a male advantage, median +0.24 sd or 4 IQ points

Table 5.5. Sex differences on the WAIS Full Scale IQ and Performance subtests; (ds; positive signs denote males score higher)

Country Test: N Full Scale IQ d  Reference 
Brazil WAIS-III: 3494 .07 Victora et al., 2015 
Canada WAIS-III: 1104 .11 Longman et al., 2007 
Chile WAIS-IV: 887 .20 Diaz & Lynn, 2016 
China WAIS-R:1406 .24 Dai et al., 1991 
China WAIS-R:1979 .33 Lynn & Dai, 1993 
China WAIS-R:120 .43 Yao et al., 2004 
China WAIS-111:888 .29 Chen & Lynn , 2020c 
China WAIS IV:311 .62 Gao et al., 2015 
Canada WAIS 111:1104 .11 Longman et al., 2007 
Denmark WAIS:62 .21 Nyborg, 2005 
Germany WAIS-IV:1425 .21 Daseking et al., 2017 
Italy  WAIS-R:1168 .45 Tommasi et al., 2015 
Japan WAIS-R:1402 .22 Hattori & Lynn, 1997 
Netherlands WAIS:2100 .27 Stinissen, 1977 
Netherlands WAIS 111:522 .24 Van der Sluis et al, 2006 
Romania  WAIS:100 .44 Dumitrascu, 1999 
Romania: Roma  WAIS:100 .44 Dumitrascu, 1999 
Russia WAIS:296 .13 Grigoriev et al, 2016 
Russia WAIS:1800 .22 Grigoriev et al, 2016 
Scotland WAIS-R:200 .39 Lynn, 1998 
South Korea  WAIS-IV:1228 .31 Lynn  & Hur, 2016 
Spain WAIS 111:1369 .24 Colom et al., 2002 
Sudan WAIS-R:330 .31 Sulman et al, 2018  
Sudan WAIS-R:319 .21 Sulman et al, 2018  
Taiwan WAIS 111:888 .29 Chen & Lynn, 2021a 
Taiwan WAIS 1V:1105 .35 Chen & Lynn, 2018 
United States W-Bell:235 .59 Strange & Palmer, 1953 
United States W-Bell:153 .20 Norman, 1953 
United States W-Bell:392 .29 Goolishian  & Foster, 1954 
United States WAIS:1700 .10 Matarazzo, 1972 
United States WAIS:279 .40 Boor, 1975 
United States WAIS:588 .17 Horn et al., 1979 
United States WAIS:521 .13 Turner & Willerman, 1977 
United States WAIS:649 .09 Doppelt & Wallace, 1955 
United States WAIS:649 .09 Doppelt & Wallace, 1955 
United States WAIS:100 .33 Shaw, 1965 
United States WAIS-R:230 .27 Arceneaux et al., 1996 
United States WAIS-R:206 .28 Ilai & Willerman, 1989 
United States WAIS-R:1880 .15 Matarazzo et al., 1986 
United States WAIS-III: 2450 .18 Irwing, 2912 
United States WAIS IV:2200 .15 Piffer, 2016 
United States WAIS 111: 850 .04 Du Pont et al., 2020 
Median   .24  

Raven’s Progressive Matrices

32 of the 33 studies found a male advantage, median 0.3 SD or 4.5 IQ points

Table 4.3. Sex differences (ds) for the Standard and Advanced Progressive Matrices in general population samples aged 20-80 years. Minus signs denote higher means obtained by females.

Country N:MN:FdReference
Britain 300240 .29Heron & Chown, 1967 
Hungary 250250 .17Szegedi, 1974 
Israel 100100 .31Guttman, 1974. 
Hawaii 939971 .37Wilson et al., 1975 
Taiwan 225 225 1.33Adair & Pollitt, 1985  
Belgium 850979 .31Deltour, 1993 
Belgium 101174 .38Deltour, 1993 
USA 63  80 .16Sitkei & Michael, 1996 
Belgium ** 564802 .21Dufouil et al., 1997 
Brazil 1921741 .28Campos, 1999 
USA** 92114 .31Salthouse, 2001 
Scotland 210217 .11Deary et al., 2004 
Scotland 230313 .29Deary et al., 2004 
Guatemala 683786 .52Martorell et al., 2005  
Brazil** 104265 .49Rossetti et al., 2009  
Pakistan 9971019 .04Ahmad et al., 2008  
Morocco  92110 .38Sellami et al., 2010  
Spain  101157 .15Diaz et al., 2010.  
Sudan 115125 .12Khaleefa et al., 2010 
N. Zealand 143187 .22Fletcher & Hattie, 2011 
Argentina  374390 -.02Flynn, 2012 
Libya 300300 .37Al-Shahomee, 2012 
Libya 260260 .36Al-Shahomee & Lynn, 2012 
Brazil** 454534 .10Flores-Mendoza et al., 2013 
Brazil**  161386 .65Braga et al., 2014 
Serbia 6274 .27Čvorović & Lynn, 2014 
Romania* 618823 .18Iliescu et al., 2016 
Australia**   128327 .30Waschl et al, 2016  
Brazil**  381216 .43Flores-Mendoza et al., 2016 
USA*** 393503 .21Van der Linden et al., 2017 
Poland** 218218 .12Gignac & Zajenkowski, 2019 
USA*** 346399 .05Du Pont et al., 2020  
Portugal 250272 .34Queiro-Garcia et al., 2021 
Median – .30 –

 *Progressive Matrices Plus; **Advanced Progressive Matrices; *** Advanced Progressive Matrices Short Form 

Brain Size

Boys and girls have about the same IQ up to 15 years. From the age of 16 years the average IQ of males becomes higher with an advantage increasing to approximately 4 IQ points in adulthood.

The reason for this is that the brain size of males increases relative to that of females from the age of 16 shown by Lenroot, Gogtay, Greenstein, Wells et al. (2007) and Lenroot & Giedd (2010) and confirmed by neurological studies that have shown that white matter in the brain continues to grow more in males than in females from mid-adolescence (Simmonds, Hallquist, Asato & Luna, 2014; Wang, Adamson, Yuan, Altaye, Rajagopal, Byars & Holland, 2012). This thesis has been further supported by studies showing that males have later physical maturation (Hills & Byrne, 2010), behavioural maturation (Greenstein, Blachstein & Vakil, 2010; Keulers, Evers, Stiers & Jollies, 2010; Yurgelun-Todd, 2007), brain maturation (De Bellis, Keshavan, Beers et al., 2001) and brain development shown by neuroimaging by Bramen, Hranilovich, Dahl et al. (2010) and by Tiemeier, Lenroot, Greenstein et al. (2010). 

The effect of the increasing brain size of males compared with that of females from the age of 16 years is that male intelligence increases relative to that of females.

Table 2.1. Sex differences in intelligence at ages 12 through 21 (ds; positive signs denote males score higher). AR: abstract reasoning.

Age12131415161718192021Reference
Female per cent  brain size92.292.592.691.591.289.286.6Roche & Malina, 1983; Rushton, 1992
Sex diffs brain size cc110120127133140150160170 Giedd et al., 2012
US: AR0.04.09.10.16Feingold, 1988
UK: AR.06.08.08.19.25Lynn, 1992
Spain: AR.05-.20..14.31.32.38.36Colom & Lynn, 2004
US: whites: g-.03.26.29.17.23.32.41Meisenberg, 2009
US: blacks: g-.11.07.05.07.00.10.10Meisenberg, 2009
UK: g-.06.12Lynn & Kanazawa, 2011
US:DAT-.80.60-3Keith et al., 2011
US: CogAT-.03-.01-.01.03Lakin, 2013
US: whites: g.08.10..02.16.23.26Nyborg, 2015
US: blacks: g-.13-.18-.04-.19-.34.43Nyborg, 2015
US: Hispanics: g.02.11-.08.24-.23.30Nyborg, 2015
Spain: IQ.07.01.08.19.27.25.32Arribas-Agula et al., 2019  
Spain: Verbal  ability-.03-.07-.06.05.15.09.15Arribas-Agula et al.,  2019

Row 1 gives the cranial capacity of females as a % of that of males. Cranial capacity of females as a percentage of that of males declines from the ages of 15 to 17 (data from Roche & Malina, 1983) and declines further at age 21+.

Row 2 gives the differences in brain size in cubic centimetres between males and females obtained by magnetic imaging : the differences increase over the ages 12 through 19.

Rows 3, 4 and 5 give data for Abstract Reasoning from the Differential Aptitude Test for the United States, the United Kingdom and Spain all showing a male advantage from ages 14 through 18.

Row 6 gives results for 15- to 21-year-old whites for the ASVAB (Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery) scored for g, showing a female advantage at age 15 followed by increasing male advantages from age 16 reaching .41d (6.15 IQ points) at age 21.

Row 7 gives results from the same data for Blacks, also showing a female advantage at age 15 followed by male advantages from age 16 but these are very small and not statistically significant (smaller sex differences in intelligence among Africans, see below the evolutionary explanation of sex differences and sex differences).

Row 8 gives the differences in g for a UK longitudinal sample, showing girls at age 12 had a significantly higher score than boys but at age 16 the same boys had a significantly higher score than girls.

Row 9 gives results for the American DAT averaged from Gv (visual-spatial) (verbal), Glr (free recall memory),Gsm (short-term memory) and Gs (processing speed), showing girls scored higher at age 12 (also at ages 5-7 and 8-10) while boys scored higher at ages 14-15 and 16-17.

Row 10 gives results for the American CogAT averaged from verbal, quantitative and non-verbal reasoning ability, showing girls scored higher at age 12, 13 and 15 while boys scored higher at age 17.

Rows 11 through 13 give results for the NLSY 97 (National Longitudinal Study of Youth) scored for g for whites, blacks and Hispanics, showing generally small sex differences from ages 12 through 15 but significant male advantages at age 17.

Row 14 gives results for g (general intelligence) for Spain, showing no significant sex differences at ages 12 through 14, and increasingly higher scores by boys from ages 15 through 18.

Row 15 gives results for verbal ability for Spain, showing girls scored higher at age 12. In a Spanish sample there was a female advantage in 12- through 14-year-olds and male advantages from the age of 15 through 18 years of .05d increasing to .15d.

To calculate the magnitude of the higher adult male IQ that would be predicted from the larger male brain size I took Ankney’s figure of the male-female difference in brain size expressed in standard deviation units of 0.78d and Willerman et al.’s (1991) estimate of the correlation between brain size and intelligence of 0.35. These figures would give adult males a higher average IQ of 4.0 IQ points (0.78 multiplied by 0.35 = .27d = 4.0 IQ).

The Evolution of Sex Differences in Intelligence 

In infants aged between 1 and 4 years, girls have higher average intelligence than boys, between the ages of  6 and 15 years there is virtually no difference in intelligence between males and females, while at the age of 16 years males begin to have higher average intelligence than females increasing an advantage of 4 IQ points in adults.

 The explanation of the higher intelligence of girls than of boys in infancy is that girls mature earlier than boys, as shown by Lenroot, Gogtay, Greenstein et al. (2007) in a longitudinal investigation of sex differences in brain development using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of 387 subjects aged 3 to 27 years, showing that cerebral volume peaked at age 10.5 in females and 14.5 in males. The likely evolutionary explanation of earlier maturation of girls is that it is advantageous for them to begin reproducing in early puberty when they are sufficiently mature to have babies and look after them. Between the ages of  6 and 15 years, the greater brain size of boys compensates for their later maturation resulting in virtually no difference in average intelligence between males and females during these ages.   

The likely evolutionary explanation of the higher intelligence of males from the age of 16 years is that in all mammalian group-living species, males compete for territory or high status in dominance hierarchies to secure access to females and reproduction as documented in detail by Wynne-Edwards (1962) and Wilson (1975). During the evolution of the hominids, greater intelligence would have contributed to success in this male competition by enabling males with greater intelligence to form useful alliances, display leadership qualities in hunting and warfare, and to dominate other males with lesser intelligence. The continued maturation and increasing intelligence of males in later adolescence and into adulthood from the age of 16 years would have enabled males to acquire the experience and skills required to work their way up the dominance hierarchies and obtain sufficient status to secure access to females. In contrast, females do not need to acquire these skills. The advantage of  intelligence is present in contemporary societies where it is a significant determinant of rank indexed by socio-economic status as documented in the United States by Jencks (1972), who showed they are correlated at 0.46, and in Britain by Nettle (2003) and Saunders (2012, 2019).  

      A further probable evolutionary explanation for the higher average intelligence of men than of women lies in sexual selection, the process by which females tend to favour males with high intelligence and accept them as mates because they consider them likely to be good providers for themselves and their children. This theory was first advanced and described as sexual selection by Charles Darwin (1971) to explain why in most species males are bigger and stronger than females. Darwin argued that males have to compete with each other for mates, and females tend to accept those who are bigger and stronger and, in humans, more intelligent, with the result that “man has ultimately become superior to woman”. This thesis has been elaborated by Geoffrey Miller (2000) who writes: “Male nightingales sing more and male peacocks display more impressive visual ornaments. Male humans sing more and talk more in public gatherings, and produce more paintings and architecture… Men write more books. Men give more lectures. Men ask more questions after lectures. Men dominate mixed-sex committee discussions”.  He argues that these are strategies successfully used by intelligent males to attract females.  

         The higher average intelligence of males than of females is greater in Europeans than in sub-Saharan Africans. This was shown by Jensen & Johnson (1994) who reported that among 7-year-old whites boys had a 1.1 higher WISC IQ than girls, but among blacks girls had a 0.6 higher IQ than boys. This difference was confirmed by Meisenberg (2009) who reported that among 20- and 21-year-olds the white male advantage was .356d but the black male advantage was only .10d. The difference was further confirmed by Nyborg (2015) who reported a female advantage of .24d on g in black 16-17 year-olds and a male advantage of .30d in whites. The same difference was reported for mathematical problem-solving ability, given as .23d for blacks and .41d for whites in the meta-analysis by Hyde, Fennema & Lamon (1990). Consistent with these results, Rushton (1992) reported that the male-female difference in brain size is greater in whites than in blacks. He reported that for enlisted military personnel the male-female difference in brain size was 204 cc. in whites and 189 cc. in blacks and for officers it was 210 cc. in whites and 197 cc. in blacks.   

The likely evolutionary explanation for the greater advantage of males over females in intelligence in Europeans than in sub-Saharan Africans is that in the tropical and sub-tropical evolutionary environment of sub-Saharan Africa, black males had only a weak need for greater intelligence than females because plant and insect foods were available throughout the year and females could collect these for themselves and their children without the support of males. The effect of this was that males were only under weak selection pressure to evolve the higher intelligence required to hunt for animal foods. When early humans migrated into Europe, they found that plant and insect foods were not available in the winter and spring and males had to hunt large animals to obtain food for themselves and their females and children. Hunting large animals is more cognitively demanding than gathering plant and insect foods, so European males came under selection pressure to evolve higher intelligence than females. In confirmation of this explanation, it has been shown by Humphreys, Lin & Fleishman (1976) that white males have significantly greater hunting ability than have white females, compared with that of black males and females.   

If this theory is correct, the male intelligence advantage should be greater in North East Asians than in Europeans because North East Asians experienced a harsher and more cognitively demanding environment. This expectation is confirmed by the sex differences in the Wechsler Full IQs in the 45 adult samples given in Tables 5.4 and 5.5. Eight of these samples were North East Asians (four from China, two from Taiwan, one from Japan and one from South Korea) in whom the median male advantage was .31d . The other thirty-seven were Europeans in whom the median  male advantage was .21d.  

References for the whole page

Lynn R. (2021) ‘Sex Differences in Intelligence, The Developpemental Theory’

Lynn R. (2015) ‘Race differences in intelligence. An evolutionary Analysis‘ Chapiter 16

Ian J. Deary (2006) ‘Age and sex differences in reaction time in adulthood, results from the United Kingdom health and lifestyle survey’, Psychology and aging

Van Der Linden D., Curtis S.D, Madison G. (2017) ‘Sex differences in brain size and general intelligence (g)’, Intelligence

Lynn R., Irwing P. (2006) ‘Intelligence: Is there a sex difference in IQ scores ?’ Nature 442, E1, doi:10.1038/nature04966

Arribas D., Aguilaa F. et al. (2019) ‘Testing the developmental theory of sex differences in intelligence using latent modeling: Evidence from the TEA Ability Battery (BAT-7)

Lynn R. (2017) ‘Sex Differences in Intelligence. Reply to Comments’ Mankind Quarterly 58:1145-156.

APPENDIX

Summary sex differences in intelligence Lynn